- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Prenatal exposure to air pollution during the early and middle stages of pregnancy is associated with adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes at ages 1 to 3 years
Environmental Health volume 23, Article number: 95 (2024)
Abstract
Background
A large body of data shows that fetal brain development is vulnerable to disruption by air pollution experienced by the mother during pregnancy, adversely affecting cognitive and psychomotor capabilities during childhood (De Asis-Cruz et al., Biol Psychiatry 7:480–90, 2022; Morgan ZEM et al., Environ Health 22:11, 2023). This study has sought to identify gestational windows of susceptibility to prenatal exposure to air pollution.
Methods
470 African American and Latina mother/child pairs participated in a prospective cohort study based in the low-income communities of Northern Manhattan and the South Bronx, New York City. Gestational exposure to respirable particulate matter (PM2.5) and nitrogen dioxide (NO2) was assessed through validated models in relation to cognitive and motor development assessed at ages 1, 2, and 3 years using the Bayley-II Scales. Multiple linear regression models and distributed lag models (DLM) were used to identify critical windows of exposure by trimester and week of pregnancy.
Results
By linear regression, average exposures to NO2 during the first and second trimesters and the entire pregnancy were significantly and negatively associated with the mental developmental index (MDI) at age 1. Average exposures to PM2.5 during the second trimester and the entire pregnancy were also significantly, inversely associated with age 1 MDI. No significant associations were found between these exposures and MDI at age 2. NO2 exposure during the first trimester was significantly negatively associated with MDI at age 3. Using DLM, exposures to NO2 at lags 29–30 weeks (within the first trimester) and PM2.5 at lags 17–18 weeks (second trimester) were significantly and inversely associated with MDI at age 1. Significant, inverse associations were found between exposures to NO2 at lag 29 weeks and PM2.5 at lags 27–29 weeks and children’s MDI at age 3. No significant associations were found between psychomotor index (PDI) and prenatal exposures to NO2 or PM2.5 at ages 1, 2 or 3.
Conclusions
Our finding that prenatal exposure to air pollution in the first and second trimesters was associated with lower scores for cognitive development at ages 1 and 3 is of concern because of the potential consequences of these outcomes for long-term functioning. They underscore the need for stronger policies to protect pregnant individuals and offspring, particularly during vulnerable, early life-stage of development.
Background
The prevalence of neurodevelopmental disorders (NDD), which include learning and intellectual disabilities, has risen in recent decades, reaching 19% among U.S. children aged 3–17 in 2019 and 2020 [1, 2]. While improved awareness, screening, and diagnosis have contributed to this increase, environmental factors have been found to play an important role. These include prenatal exposures to combustion-related air pollutants—fine particulate matter (PM2), nitrogen dioxide (NO2), and polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons (PAH)—that have been linked to adverse effects on early brain development, including poorer cognitive and psychomotor development, IQ reduction, attention problems, attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) behaviors, and structural changes in children’s brains [3,4,5,6]. In the U.S., there is a pattern of disproportionate exposure to air pollutants in communities of color and low income communities [7].
The focus on the prenatal period of development reflects the large body of data showing that the highly choreographed and complex processes during fetal development are vulnerable to disruption by air pollution, other toxic exposures, and stress experienced by the mother during pregnancy, adversely affecting cognitive and psychomotor capabilities during infancy and childhood [3, 8].
The Distributed Lag Model (DLM) has been used by other investigators to identify critical windows of pre- and post-natal exposure to air pollution during with respect to early child development [8,9,10,11,12]. The studies differ regarding the age at which children were assessed, the methods of assessment; and the sensitive periods identified. Several prior studies of the effects of air pollution have suggested that the mid- to- late gestational periods may be critical windows of exposure [3, 12]. Only one study assessed mental and psychomotor development using the Bayley/ Scales of Infant and Toddler Development [3]. Here, we have used DLM to examine the relationship between prenatal exposure to PM2.5 and NO2 on mental and psychomotor development at ages 1, 2 and 3 assessed using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development – Second Edition (BSID-II) in 470 Latino and African American mother-infant pairs in New York City (NYC). Our goal was to identify vulnerable periods of exposure to inform and support early interventions to reduce exposure to air pollution.
Methods
The cohort
Data are drawn from the Columbia Center for Children’s Environmental Health (CCCEH) Mothers and Newborns prospective cohort study, previously described [13, 14]. The participants resided in communities in Northern Manhattan and the South Bronx, which are low-income communities directly served by the Mailman School of Public Health and CCCEH. The Mothers and Newborns cohort includes self-identified African American and Latina women aged 18 – 35 recruited between 1998 and 2006. Women were excluded who initiated prenatal care after the 20th week of pregnancy, had a multiple pregnancy, used tobacco products or illicit drugs, had diabetes or hypertension, were HIV positive, or had not resided in New York City for at least a year. Prenatal visits were conducted in the third trimester of pregnancy (on average at 39.3 weeks of gestation) and children with birth anomalies were not excluded from continuing with the study. Protocols for the cohort were approved by the Columbia University Medical Center Institutional Review Board under protocol number AAAA6110.
Modeling of exposure based on residential address
For each mother/newborn pair, daily exposure to PM2.5 and NO2 at participants’ residences during pregnancy was estimated using validated spatio-temporal models described previously [15, 16]. The models incorporated data from the New York City Community Air Survey (NYCCAS), provided by the New York City Department of Health and Mental Hygiene staff, and regulatory data from the Environmental Protection Agency’s Air Quality System (AQS) (https://aqsdr1.epa.gov/aqsweb/aqstmp/airdata/download_files.html). The amount/density of candidate spatial predictors were computed, such as traffic and land use-related variables (derived from the New York City Department of City Planning’s taxlot database) within 4 different buffer areas around each monitoring site: 100, 300, 500 and 1000 m. Several temporal predictors (temperature, relative humidity, and wind speed) and the mixing depth and air-mass trajectory for each day were included in the models. The level of exposure to each of the two pollutants was estimated for each day of the pregnancy at a spatial resolution of 25 × 25 m2. The average values for each week, trimester, and the entire pregnancy were calculated for all mother–child pairs.
Children’s neurodevelopmental outcomes
We used the Bayley Scales of Infant Development–Second Edition (BSID-II) to assess cognitive and psychomotor development at 12, 24, and 36 months of age (Bayley 1993). The BSID-II is the most widely used norm-referenced developmental test for young children. The raw score is used to determine the index score for each measure, based off the child’s exact age at assessment, generating the mental development index (MDI) and psychomotor development index (PDI) scores. Both the MDI and PDI scores range from 50–150, with lower scores indicating more delays compared to same age peers and higher scores indicate the child is performing above the level of same age peers.
Statistical methods
Data on prenatal exposures
We extracted daily exposure measures of NO2 and PM2.5 during pregnancy for 727 mothers. We then calculated average measures of exposure for the first, second, and third trimesters, and the entire pregnancy, as well as for each week of pregnancy. The first trimester spans gestational week 1 to week 13, the second trimester week 14 to week 26, and the third trimester week 27 to DOB.
Multiple linear regression to test the association between NO2/PM2.5 exposure during different periods of pregnancy and children’s neurodevelopmental outcomes
We conducted linear regression models to test for associations between prenatal NO2/PM2.5 exposure in each trimester and neurodevelopmental performance composite scores (MDI/PDI) adjusting for gender, mother’s ethnicity, maternal self-report of environmental tobacco smoke (ETS) exposure, maternal education (< high school, \(\ge\) high school), maternal intelligence measured by the Test of Non-verbal Intelligence version 2 (TONI-2), and maternal demoralization. Maternal demoralization is measured using the Psychiatric Epidemiology Research Instrument Demoralization Scale, which contains 27 questions. Each question was rated on a scale from 0 to 4 with higher scores indicating greater psychological distress and participants were queried about symptoms within the previous year. The maternal demoralization score is the average score for the 27 questions. [15]. At age 3, a measure of the quality of the early home caretaking environment (HOME) was available. The Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) is an instrument that can be used to measure a child’s home environment, including the quality of cognitive stimulation and emotional support provided by the child’s family, which contains 8 scales including learning material, language stimulation, physical environment, responsivity, academic stimulation, modeling, variety and acceptance. The home score is the total scores of the 8 scales. [16] We observed that adjusting for this variable changed most of the exposure-outcome associations at age 3 by more than 10% (Supplemental Tables S5-S6). Thus, we included HOME as an additional covariate in analysis of MDI and PDI at age 3.
We examined the effects of exposures to NO2 and PM2.5 during the first, second, and third trimesters, and the entire pregnancy, respectively, on MDI and PDI scores at ages 1, 2, and 3. For MDI or PDI outcomes, we conducted a total of 24 tests (4 pregnancy time periods, 2 exposures, and 3 ages). We calculated false discovery rate (FDR) with the Benjamini-Hochberg (BH) procedure and used 0.05 as the threshold to adjust for multiple comparisons with 24 tests.
Distributed lag models to identify critical windows of exposure to NO2/PM2.5
We conducted DLM to examine the relationship between weekly exposure to NO2 and PM2.5 during pregnancy and MDI and PDI at ages 1, 2, and 3. In DLM, exposure during the week of delivery is termed lag 0; exposure during the week before the delivery is termed lag 1, and so on. Mothers with different gestational durations will have a different number of lags. To be consistent, we considered the first 35 gestational weeks for all mothers. To evaluate the relationships between weekly exposures during pregnancy and neurodevelopmental outcomes using DLM, we adjusted for the same covariates as in the linear regression models. We used a B-spline with 3 degrees of freedom to account for correlations among the 35 gestational weeks and used R package ‘DLNM’. For each of the neurodevelopmental outcomes, we conducted 6 tests (2 exposures and 3 ages). Therefore, we used Bonferroni correction to adjust for multiple comparisons and obtained confidence intervals (CIs) on lag coefficients from the DLM models. Specifically, we obtained the coefficient estimates of the lag vectors and their variances and the corresponding CIs with the Bonferroni-adjusted significance level 0.05/6; i.e., 99% CI (1–0.05/6 = 0.9917). If this CI did not include 0, we concluded that there was statistical significance after adjustment for multiple comparisons.
Results
Figure 1 shows the numbers of participants enrolled and those included in the analysis at each age. A total of 520 mother–child pairs had available exposure data and main covariates. The mother–child pairs were further restricted based on availability of MDI and PDI data at ages 1, 2, and 3 years. We compared pairs included in the analyses at each age, with respect to associations between MDI and exposures, to those not included because they were missing main covariates. The detailed results are provided in supplementary materials (Supplemental Tables S1-S3). For MDI at age 1, a higher proportion of the excluded group of mothers and children were Latina compared to the included group. For MDI at age 2, there was no significant difference between the included and excluded groups. For MDI at age 3, the excluded group had a higher maternal intelligence score and lower NO2 and PM2.5 exposure levels compared to the included group. Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics of covariate variables and the prenatal exposure level of PM2.5 and NO2.
Association between NO2 and PM2.5 and infant or child neurodevelopmental outcomes by linear regression
Using linear regression models, we found that MDI at age 1 was negatively associated with several measures of prenatal exposure to NO2 and PM2.5 (Table 2). After adjustment for multiple comparisons, average exposure to NO2 during the first, second trimester and entire pregnancy were significantly and negatively associated with MDI at age 1 (with a BH adjusted p-value < 0.05). Average exposure to PM2.5 during the second trimester and the entire pregnancy had significant negative associations with MDI at age 1. No significant associations were found between children’s MDI or PDI at age 2 and prenatal exposure to NO2 or PM2.5. In the analysis of children’s MDI at age 3, we further adjusted for the quality of the home caretaking environment (HOME) as noted above. Exposure to NO2 during the first trimester remained negatively associated with MDI (BH adjusted p-value = 0.053) (Table 2).
Comparison analysis between the group without HOME at age 3 and the group included in the MDI age 3 analysis suggested that the variable indicating whether the mother completed a high school education was significantly different between the two groups. Therefore, we conducted a sensitivity analysis using inverse probability weighting (IPW) to investigate the influence of excluding the participants without HOME in the MDI age 3 analysis. Specifically, we pooled these two groups (the mother/child pairs without HOME and those with HOME, the latter being included in the MDI age 3 analysis) and calculated the probability of each pair being included in the analysis, using all variables except HOME. We used the inverse of this probability to weight participants to account for the fact that the percentage of participants having completed high school was significantly different between the two groups. We observed that the results from analysis with IPW were very similar to the results without IPW (Supplemental Table S4). Therefore, we concluded that the difference between the two groups did not influence our result.
No significant associations were found between PDI and prenatal exposures of NO2 and PM2.5 after FDR correction (Table 3). We also explored whether there were sex-specific associations. No differences were discovered between males and females.
Distributed lag models to test the effects of weekly exposures of NO2 and PM2.5 during pregnancy
Figures 2 and 3 display the effects of weekly exposures to NO2 or PM2.5 during pregnancy on MDI or PDI at ages 1, 2, and 3, after multiple comparisons adjustment. We observed significant, negative lag effects (with Bonferroni-corrected 99.2% CIs not including 0) of NO2 and PM2.5 on MDI at ages 1 and 3, and a positive lag effect of exposure to PM2.5 on PDI at age 3.
Table 4 shows the significant lag effects of NO2 and PM2.5 on MDI and PDI with effect estimates and Bonferroni-adjusted 99.2% CIs. For MDI at age 1, exposure to NO2 at lags 29–30 had significant, negative effects (Table 4). This was consistent with the results obtained from the regression models where exposure of NO2 during the first trimester was negatively associated with children’s MDI at age 1 (Table 2) because lags 29–30 are within the first trimester of pregnancy. Exposure to PM2.5 at lags 17–18 also had significantly negative effects on MDI at age 1 (Table 4) which is consistent with the regression results where the average exposure to PM2.5 during the second trimester was negatively associated with MDI at age 1 (Table 2). Exposures of NO2 at lag 29 and PM2.5 at lags 27–29 had significantly negative effects on children’s MDI at age 3 (Table 4), which were not revealed in the regression model. With respect to PDI, although exposures of PM2.5 during lags 12–13 had significant associations with PDI, the 99.2% CIs almost include 0.
Discussion
In a large sample of mother/child pairs drawn from a prospective cohort of color (African American and Latina) residing in Northern Manhattan and the South Bronx, we have found significant, negative associations between prenatal exposure to two combustion-related pollutants (PM2.5 and NO2) on cognitive (MDI) development at ages 1 and 3 years. Using multiple linear regressions, we found significant, negative associations between MDI at age 1 and average exposure to NO2 during the first and second trimesters and the entire pregnancy. Average exposure to PM2.5 during the second trimester and the entire pregnancy had significant negative effects on MDI at age 1. MDI at age 3 was negatively and significantly associated with exposure to NO2 during the first trimester. No significant associations with MDI were seen at age 2. No significant associations were found between PDI and exposure to NO2 or PM2.5. The inconsistent results between the outcomes of age 2 and ages 1 or 3 may be due to the decrease we have previously observed in the present cohort in the mean cognitive scores at two years of age [8]. Such a decline on the Bayley-II at two years has been previously reported in low-income families and communities, independent of chemical exposures [1,2,3], and may contribute to our lack of significant association at 24 months [17,18,19,20].
Demonstrating the utility of DLM to identify possible critical windows of exposure on a finer scale, we found significant lag effects of NO2 and PM2.5 on MDI. For MDI at age 1, exposure to NO2 at lags 29–30 (i.e., weeks within the first trimester) had significant, negative effects, as did PM2.5 exposure at lags 17–18 (weeks within the second trimester). With respect to MDI at age 3, exposure to NO2 at lag 29 and PM2.5 at lags 27–29 (first trimester) had significant, negative effects. Many of these associations agree with what we observed using linear regressions based on trimester of exposure, while several of these associations were not revealed using linear regressions based on trimester of exposure.
The results for PDI were non-significant except for a borderline significant, positive association with exposure to PM2.5 during lags 12–13.
Our results in mothers and newborns in NYC contrast with those of a study in 161 Latino mother-infant pairs in Southern California using DLM that pointed to the mid- to-late gestational periods as windows of susceptibility to the effects of PM2.5 on cognitive and motor development at age 2. The authors did not assess associations at ages 1 and 3 [3]. The lack of significant findings at age 2 in our study, in contradiction to theirs, may be due to differences in the characteristics of the study populations, exposure levels, and statistical methods, including differing covariates. We also used a more stringent adjustment for multiple comparisons.
Suggested mechanisms for the observed negative effects on neurodevelopment are not fully understood. However, PM2.5 and NO2, two major components of air pollution, have been found to contribute to elevated oxidative stress, leading to inflammatory responses, DNA damage, and disruption of cellular signaling pathways [14]. A more complete mechanistic discussion is beyond the scope of the work, but others have considered the impact on different brain-based developmental processes on gestation [8].
Limitations of the present study include the fact that our cohort includes only African American and Latina women of lower income, so our results may not be generalizable to populations of other ethnicities and socioeconomic status. Further, we considered only PM2.5 and NO2 and were not able to assess the associations with other air pollutants. We also did not have data on postnatal exposure to these pollutants and were not able to assess their possible impact on cognitive and motor development.
Advantages of the study include our well- characterized multiethnic cohort drawn from communities of color and low income who are most at risk from air pollution, a larger sample size than previous studies, and the ability to assess effects by week of exposure as well as by trimester and entire pregnancy.
Conclusion
Our findings using linear regression and DLM suggest that the first and second trimesters, and certain weeks therein, may be vulnerable windows of exposure to air pollution, resulting in lower scores for cognitive development at ages 1 and 3. This is of concern because of the potential consequences for long-term functioning. These findings add to a large body of evidence on the vulnerability of the fetal developmental period to air pollution and underscore the need for stronger policies to protect this vulnerable life stage.
Data availability
Data available upon request.
References
Yang Y, Zhao S, Zhang M, Xiang M, Zhao J, Chen S, et al. Prevalence of neurodevelopmental disorders among US children and adolescents in 2019 and 2020. Front Psychol. 2022;24(13):997648.
Zablotsky B, Black LI, Maenner MJ, Schieve LA, Danielson ML, Bitsko RH, et al. Prevalence and trends of developmental disabilities among children in the United States: 2009–2017. Pediatrics. 2019;144(4):e20190811.
Morgan ZEM, Bailey MJ, Trifonova DI, Naik NC, Patterson WB, Lurmann FW, et al. Prenatal exposure to ambient air pollution is associated with neurodevelopmental outcomes at 2 years of age. Environ Health. 2023;22(1):11.
Chiu YHM, Hsu HHL, Coull BA, Bellinger DC, Kloog I, Schwartz J, et al. Prenatal particulate air pollution and neurodevelopment in urban children: Examining sensitive windows and sex-specific associations. Environ Int. 2016;1(87):56–65.
Liu B, Fang X, Strodl E, He G, Ruan Z, Wang X, et al. Fetal exposure to air pollution in late pregnancy significantly increases ADHD-risk behavior in early childhood. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(17):10482.
Perera F, Nadeau K. Climate change, fossil-fuel pollution, and children’s health. N Engl J Med. 2022;386(24):2303–14.
Tessum CW, Paolella DA, Chambliss SE, Apte JS, Hill JD, Marshal JD. PM2.5 polluters disproportionately and systemically affect people of color in the United States. Sci Adv. 2021;7(18):eabf4491.
De Asis-Cruz J, Andescavage N, Limperopoulos C. Adverse prenatal exposures and fetal brain development: insights from advanced fetal magnetic resonance imaging. Biol Psychiatry. 2022;7(5):480–90.
Whitworth KW, Rector-Houze AM, Chen WJ, Ibarluzea J, Swartz M, Symanski E, et al. Relation of prenatal and postnatal PM2.5 exposure with cognitive and motor function among preschool-aged children. Int J Hyg Environ Health. 2024;256:114317.
Guilbert A, Bernard JY, Peyre H, Costet N, Hough I, Seyve E, et al. Prenatal and childhood exposure to ambient air pollution and cognitive function in school-age children: xamining sensitive windows and sex-specific associations. Environ Res. 2023;15(235):116557.
Crooijmans KLHA, Iñiguez C, Withworth KW, Estarlich M, Lertxundi A, Fernández-Somoano A, et al. Nitrogen dioxide exposure, attentional function, and working memory in children from 4 to 8 years: periods of susceptibility from pregnancy to childhood. Environ Int. 2024;186:108604.
Ni Y, Loftus CT, Szpiro AA, Young MT, Hazlehurst MF, Murphy LE, et al. Associations of pre- and postnatal air pollution exposures with child behavioral problems and cognitive performance: a U.S. multi-cohort study. Environ Health Perspect. 2022;130(6):67008.
Perera FP, Tang D, Wang S, Vishnevetsky J, Zhang B, Diaz D, et al. Prenatal Polycyclic Aromatic Hydrocarbon (PAH) exposure and child behavior at age 6–7 years. Environ Health Perspect. 2012;120(6):921–6.
Wang Y, Perera F, Guo J, Riley KW, Durham T, Ross Z, et al. A methodological pipeline to generate an epigenetic marker of prenatal exposure to air pollution indicators. Epigenetics. 2022;17(1):32–40.
Ananth CV, Kioumourtzoglou MA, Huang Y, Ross Z, Friedman AM, Williams MA, et al. Exposures to air pollution and risk of acute-onset placental abruption: a case-crossover study. Epidemiology. 2018;29(5):631–8.
Huang Y, Kioumourtzoglou MA, Mittleman MA, Ross Z, Williams MA, Friedman AM, et al. Air pollution and risk of placental abruption: a study of births in New York City, 2008–2014. Am J Epidemiol. 2021;190(6):1021–33.
Rauh VA, Garfinkel R, Perera FP, Andrews HF, Hoepner L, Barr DB, et al. Impact of prenatal chlorpyrifos exposure on neurodevelopment in the first 3 years of life among inner-city children. Pediatrics. 2006;118(6):e1845–59.
Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:371–99.
Morisset CE, Barnard KE, Greenberg MT, Booth CL, Spieker SJ. Environmental influences on early language development: he context of social risk. Dev Psychopathol. 1990;2(2):127–49.
Black MM, Hess CR, Berenson-Howard J. Toddlers from low-income families have below normal mental, motor, and behavior scores on the revised bayley scales. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2000;21(6):655–66.
Acknowledgements
We would like to acknowledge James Quinn, Department of Epidemiology, Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University for work on GIS. Funding was provided by the National Institutes of Health: UG3OD023290, UH3OD023290, National Institute for Environmental Health Sciences (NIEHS) and the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (US EPA): NIEHS/EPA P01ES09600/RD82702701, NIEHS/EPAP01ES09600/RD832141, NIEHS/EPA P01ES09600/RD834509, NIEHS/EPA P50ES09600/RD83615401, NIEHS R01ES014393, NIEHS RC2ES018784, NIEHS R01ES13163, NIEHS R01ES08977, and NIEHS R24ES029489, the New York Community Trust, Trustees of the Blanchette Hooker Rockefeller Fund, and the John and Wendy Neu Foundation.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
FP, KR and JH wrote the main textand contributed to the database, YM and SW conducted statistical analyses and prepared figures and tables, ZR conducted expposure modeling, VH, VR, AM contributed to the database. All authors reviewed the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Perera, F., Miao, Y., Ross, Z. et al. Prenatal exposure to air pollution during the early and middle stages of pregnancy is associated with adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes at ages 1 to 3 years. Environ Health 23, 95 (2024). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12940-024-01132-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12940-024-01132-9